does cognitive therapy rest on a mistake?
...first draft of a psychological critique of CBT to complement the philosophical one I produced a couple of years ago for the OUP Philosophy and Psychiatry Handbook ... this one to be submitted to BJ Psych Bulletin after I've improved it... The title, by the way, is me riffing on John Heil's (1981) excellent Mind paper 'Does cognitive psychology rest on a mistake?'.
Summary
The theory and practice of cognitive therapy grew
out of Aaron Beck's dissatisfactions with psychoanalysis in the 1950s and 60s.
He replaced an understanding of depression as motivationally driven by an
avoidance of anger with one of it as incidentally maintained by negative
automatic thoughts. Along the way the aim of therapy also shifted - from the
restoration of self-possession to the achievement of self-management. In this
paper I argue: that Beck's scientific reasons for abandoning psychoanalysis
were mistaken; that his clinical reasons are best formulated psychoanalytically
rather than cognitively - in terms of the transference relationship; and that
cognitive therapy’s limited understanding of that relationship risks it
delivering an under-powered therapy.
Introduction
Aaron Beck, the founder of cognitive therapy (CT),
was a psychoanalytically-minded psychiatrist who became disaffected with the
psychoanalytic theory and therapy of depression popular in
mid-twentieth-century America. On reading and listening to his descriptions of his
own standard-issue thinking and practice as a psychoanalytical psychotherapist we
readily understand his disaffection. The psychoanalytic theory of depression he
describes appears emotionally remote and intellectually recondite. Rather than
being meaningfully grounded in and constrained by a living, phenomenologically
sensitive, emotional connection with patients’ inner worlds, it is brought to
bear on them from without, as if it were merely an independently intelligible psychological
hypothesis being used to causally explain some experiential data. The
therapeutic procedures he describes also sound disengaged; we hear nothing of
his own emotional experience of being with the patient, and little comes across
of the quality of his patients’ inner struggles, conflicts and evasions. Over
time Beck courageously challenged the validity of his own deeply held
psychoanalytic theory and practice. But rather than go on to challenge the
adequacy of his epistemological framework to frame a more meaningful and
sensitive encounter with the dynamics of his patients’ inner worlds, he instead
jettisons the very idea of the latter’s significance. Therapy now becomes just about
attentional training and intellectual disputation; occurrent emotional
connection and disconnection become not means to understand and refigure the
inner world, but matters of merely facilitative or obstructive significance to
the cognitive training. In what follows I trace one theoretical and one
clinical track from Beck’s rejection of psychoanalysis to his invention of CT
and highlight the losses to clinical theory and practice that accrue along the
way.
Depression, Anger and Dreams
An important theme in psychoanalytic
psychopathology has depression resulting from the suppression of anger toward
valued others. Rather than risk the feared fall-out from expressing his anger,
the patient unconsciously depletes himself, trading that sense of self-worth
which his angry sense of injustice presupposes for the stability of his
relationships. This idea forms but one strand of the complex psychoanalytic
conceptualisation of anger's relation to depression (Freud 1917; Busch 2009;
Lubbe 2011), but is widespread in both popular and clinical culture for good
reason. Clinically we discover, again and again in the more straightforward
cases, i) a depressed person who avoids her anger by downgrading her sense of
her own value, shutting down her self-assertion, wrongly construing herself as
perpetrator rather than victim of a relational injustice, seeing herself as
deserving of treatment which neutral others would consider unjust, and denying
the significance of her unmet emotional needs. In more complex cases however we
find ii) masochistic self-abasement added to this anger-avoiding dynamic:
hatred towards another breaks through the attempts at self-suppression and
gives rise to intolerable guilt, and this in turn inspires self-punishment
where the anger towards the other is 'retroflected' (taken out on the self)
leading to further and darker depressive misery. In clinical practice the
therapist finds the recovery of energy and mood going hand in hand with the
reinstating of apt outer-directed emotional expression, self-esteem and
assertiveness as the patient learns to tolerate his hate and guilt and address
his relational difficulties in an open, constructive and honest manner. Many
effective psychoanalytical psychotherapies base almost their entire therapeutic
rationale and method on this psychoanalytical foundation (e.g. Frederickson
2013).
It was as a reaction against the above-described
psychoanalytic theory of depression that a young, American,
psychoanalytically-minded psychiatrist and dream researcher developed the
theory and practice of cognitive therapy. Following a helpful personal
experience of psychoanalysis, Aaron Beck treated several patients using
standard mid-century psychoanalytic methods, applied (albeit unsuccessfully)
for membership of the American Psychoanalytic Institute, and published several
papers both on psychoanalytic psychotherapy and on the quantitative
investigation of the themes of his depressed patients' dreams (e.g. Beck &
Hurvich, 1959; Beck & Ward, 1961). Reading these today we learn in
particular of his scientific interest in the increased prevalence of thwarted,
deprived, excluded, rejected, injured and ashamed themes in his patients'
dreaming lives, and of his clinical interpretation of these along
psychoanalytic lines.
Already in these early papers, however, we find a
curious feature which presages his later rejection of psychoanalysis: although
most of the themes Beck describes (e.g. 'I was in a restaurant but the waiters
would not serve me'; 'Everyone was invited to the party but me'; 'My fiancée
married someone else') appear interpretable in terms of i) the simple
hypothesis of motivated self-depletion, Beck surprisingly interprets them all
in terms of ii) the more complex and compounded dynamic of self-hatred: the
depressive's misery is always seen as deliberately rather than incidentally
self-inflicted - as reflecting a 'need to suffer' (Beck 1967, p. 179).
Dreams such as not getting food that is requested, or being rejected - themes
which in themselves appear to reflect little more than a need to portray the
self as worthless relative to others - are instead counter-intuitively seen as
'the representation of self-punitive tendencies ... the depressed person feels
guilt about his ego-alien drives and punishes himself for them.' (Beck &
Hurvich, 1959, p. 54). However Beck's later experimental and clinical
investigations - including his patients' negative reactions to
interpretations that were overly organised by this prior theory-driven
hypothesis -rightly led him to doubt whether many of his patients really
were motivated by self-hatred ... but as a result he then threw out
the entirety of the baby of the venerable psychoanalytic theory of depression
along with the counter-intuitive bathwater of an over-reaching masochism hypothesis. Now
the project of providing a psychological explanation as to why the patient is
driven to think and feel and act in depressive ways is abandoned; depression collapses
into a merely habitual rut of self-maintaining negative thought, feeling and
behaviour. The question we must ask today, then, is why Beck was so compelled
by ii) the masochism hypothesis that it overrode his recognition of the
sufficiency in many cases of i) the simple psychoanalytic hypothesis of
depression as motivated self-depletion.
The answer to this is not apparent in the early
papers, but Beck's later writings provide an important clue. Here he tells us
that what he was actually trying to do in his early research was
establish the clinical psychoanalytic theory of depression on a firmer
scientific basis by providing quantitatively ascertained psychological evidence
of unconscious anger in the dreams of his depressed patients (Clark, Beck &
Alford, 1999; Beck & Beck 2012). Although he didn't report it at the time,
what he found is what has also been established since (Barrett & Loeffler,
1992): that as a group depressives have fewer themes of anger in their dreams
than non-depressed dreamers. This however puzzled him as he had understood the
Freudian idea of dreams as the 'royal road to the unconscious' (Freud, 1913) to
mean that feelings which were unacceptable to the waking patient ought to show
up straightforwardly in their dreams (Clark, Beck & Alford 1999, p. 50).
The finding of fewer rather than more angry themes in depressives' dreams
therefore appeared to contradict the psychoanalytic hypothesis of depression as
resulting from suppressed anger. But by interpreting the very dreaming
of all and any miserabilist themes by the dreamer as masochistically
motivated, Beck is for a while - until he finds independent evidence of the
implausibility of this interpretation - able to save the psychoanalytic theory
that in depression we meet with difficulties with anger.
Looking back, the most striking thing in this
story is Beck's misunderstanding of a core strand of the psychoanalytic theory
of dreaming. According to Freud (1913), dreams serve to protect the dreamer's
sleep by helping prevent her anxious recognition of unacceptable emotions, such
as anger towards loved ones, which anxiety would otherwise cause waking.
Dreaming may be understood as the processing of emotionally significant
experiences, especially of the previous day, where by 'processing' is meant the
alignment of the meaning of emotional experience with one's (realistic or
distorted) sense of self. The wish-fulfilling construction of self-diminishing
dreams can then be one way in which a dreamer can manage the anxiety caused by
what would otherwise be recognition of the perceived undeservedness of slights
and injustices. Accordingly, the 'road' to unconscious emotion provided by
dreams may be 'royal' compared with that of a mind filled with the myriad
preoccupations of waking life, but hardly so straight as to make for the ready
applicability of the theme-counting methods of empirical psychology which, as
is well known, Beck went on to make the mainstay of his scientific methodology.
To sum up so far: A central plank of CT's origin
myth has it that it developed out of an apparent scientific disconfirmation of
the clinical psychoanalytic theory of depression as a motivationally explicable
state. But in retrospect what seems most likely to have happened is that an
inappropriately simplistic quantitative methodology was deployed to assess an
unlikely theory of dreaming; that an implausibly general theory of depressive
masochism was developed to save the floundering analytic theory; and that when
this general masochism theory was dropped for good reasons the whole idea of
inner motivational dynamics - i.e. the whole idea of a depth psychology - was
jettisoned. As we will see below, the resultant psychology offers the
depressive the hope of release from an allegedly self-maintaining rut of
depressive thought and feeling through the effortful achievement of cognitive
self-mastery. At the same time it eschews the promise of helping patients find
motivational meaning in their suffering, reinstate a relatively effortless
self-possession, and enjoy the consequent reversal of self-diminishment.
NATs and the Transference
As described above the development of CT's
psychopathological theory rests on its rejection of the prevalent
psychoanalytic notion of depression as unconsciously motivated. The development
of CT's psychotherapeutic technique, however, rests on its rejection of the
centrality for psychotherapeutic practice of what psychoanalysis terms the
'transference relationship'. A curious aspect, then, of Beck's development of
CT is that it was actually inspired by his encounter with, and dawning
realisation of the clinical significance of, his patients' transferences to
him.
First a note on 'transference': A defining
preoccupation of psychoanalysis is with how our immersion in relationships
which inspire concern and attachment - such as those with
psychotherapists, partners, parents, employers etc. - so readily elicits
unrealistic fearful and idealising expectations concerning others' views of us.
These relentlessly maintained emotionally charged expectations are seen by
psychoanalysis as at the root of much psychopathology, and their manifestations
are known as negative and positive transferences. They can be easy to attend
from, as it were, but powerfully difficult to attend to - i.e. they are often
unconscious, and their patterns are typically transferred from one significant
other to another over time (Fried, Crits-Christoph & Luborsky 1992;
Andersen & Chen 2002). The task of psychoanalytic therapy is the patient's
emancipation from distorting transference patterns, a task prosecuted by first
facilitating the flourishing and then the subsequent emancipation from the
transferences between patient and psychotherapist, an experience that may then
generalise to the transferences in the rest of the patient's life. Much of a
psychoanalytical psychotherapist's training has to do with cultivating his
ability to be emotionally touched, yet not inexorably swept along, by the
patient's transference so he can think about, experience, describe, and help
liberate the patient from, her unconscious emotional habits.
To return to CT: As Beck tells the story (Beck
& Beck 2012), he had a patient who would lie on the couch each session and
regale him with lurid tales of her sexual exploits, whilst he sat back and
offered somewhat by-the-book psychoanalytic interpretations of the content of
whatever it had occurred to her to say. At the end of one session however
Beck's humanity got the better of his method and he asked her how she was
feeling; she replied 'anxious'. Beck first suggested, in an interpretation
focusing only on intra-psychic issues, that conflicts about sex were making her
anxious. She responded, however, to the effect that her real worry was not
intra-psychic but inter-personal: her worry was that he was bored by her.
From this Beck describes how he began to see 'that there's a whole stream of
pre-conscious thinking that goes on that the patient doesn't normally
communicate to the analyst' (Beck & Beck 2012) - especially pessimistic,
biased, black and white, over-general irrational thoughts about what the
therapist thinks and feels about the patient. Beck came to call these
transference expectations the patient's 'negative automatic thoughts' (NATs)
and, drawing on the ‘rational therapy’ of Albert Ellis (1962), went on to
develop a significant range of procedures to help the patient attend to and
challenge their NATs.
There are several striking things about this
narrative. The first is that Beck the psychoanalyst appears to have been
practicing a rather remote form of psychotherapy which was more concerned to
offer theory-derived hypotheses about the content of what the patient said,
than to emotionally engage with how and why she said it or listen out for what
she wasn't saying. The result was on the one hand that, despite her clear lack
of inhibition in talking about sex, Beck still interpreted her anxiety as due
to sexual conflict, and, on the other, that we hear nothing about what it felt
like to Beck to engage with this particular patient (his countertransference).
The second is that it is when Beck shows a real interest in his patient, actually
asking her how she feels, that she is able to acknowledge her transference to
him, and they can understand it together. We do not hear whether her anxiety
about boring Beck was consonant with her everyday anxieties about what, say,
other men thought of her, but we do at least arrive at a moment of emotional
connection between the two of them. As the therapeutic relationship is
strengthened (by Beck's concerned question about her actual feelings), the
emotionally alive experience of the transference (her worries about what Beck
thought of her) also begins to be acknowledged and worked through, and remote
speculation about intra-psychic conflict is thankfully foregone.
The third striking thing about Beck's narrative,
however, is that no sooner is this therapy-potentiating emotional experience of
the transferential context encountered, than it is set aside. Anyone who has
been in psychotherapy will know how replete it is with holding back
acknowledgement of one's thoughts and impulses for fear of the therapist's
disapproval, despite such fears speaking right to the heart of the emotional
difficulties which brought one to therapy in the first place. Notwithstanding
the simplicity of the 'fundamental rule' of psychoanalysis to 'free associate'
- i.e. say whatever is actually on our mind - the fact is that no-one can
actually follow it (Lear, 2014, p. 83), since we naturally associate away from
rather than towards conflictual emotional preoccupations (Frederickson 1999, p.
169). This is why the therapist's job is often to listen not so much to the
content of what is said as to performative matters of style, timing and
omission. Beck, however, construes NATs as merely incidentally hard for the
patient to articulate and challenge - due to a lack of attentional training and
rational ability – rather than because of their emotional valence. It is in
part because of this that CT risks providing a significantly underpowered
therapy - one that substitutes a coaching relationship for an encounter which,
by keeping itself on the experiential pulse of emotions alive in the room
between patient and therapist, is itself therapeutic.
Cognitive therapists are often accused of
downplaying the importance of the therapeutic relationship, but as psychologist
Judith Beck (2010) explains, this is false; cognitive therapy 'requires a good
therapeutic relationship. Therapists do many things to build a strong alliance.
For example, they work collaboratively with clients ... ask for feedback ...
and conduct themselves as genuine, warm, empathic, interested, caring human
beings.' However, as psychoanalytical psychotherapist Jonathan Shedler (2015)
replies, ‘This is the kind of relationship I would expect from my hair stylist
or real estate broker. From a psychotherapist, I expect something else. [Beck
appears] to have no concept that the therapy relationship provides a special
window into the patient’s inner world, or a relationship laboratory and
sanctuary in which lifelong patterns can be recognized and understood, and new
ones created.’ Shedler's optimism regarding his hair stylist and estate agent
perhaps warrants some cognitive restructuring, but his point about the
therapeutic relationship stands. A relationship which is not merely
instrumentally useful (as in CT), but itself intended as a unique vehicle of
change (as in psychoanalytical therapy), is one which both activates the
patient's latent transference fears (that the therapist is untrustworthy,
angry, etc.) whilst simultaneously providing enough of a working alliance to
challenge such fears in real time. With a merely collaborative and empathic
focus the opportunity will be lost for a powerful eliciting in the present of
the patient's true emotional preoccupations. The result is rather like trying
to conduct a behavioural exposure therapy for a phobia without physically
encountering the fearful stimulus, or like a chat between two adults about the
difficulties of a child left waiting in the next room. Without the negative
transference being activated and understood in the room, the prospect of an
intrinsically, rather than merely instrumentally, therapeutic relationship is
lost. [footnote: A prevalent myth about psychoanalysis and CT is that the
latter focuses on the present whereas psychoanalysis unhelpfully spends an
unnecessary amount of time looking at a patient's childhood. The reality,
however, is that whilst both therapies attempt at times to understand the
present in terms of the past, the focus in CT is often only on what has been
happening over the last week, whereas in psychoanalysis it is largely on the
patient's here-and-now live emotional experience of the therapist.]
Conclusions: Self-Management and
Self-Possession
Therapies of all stripes aim at furthering
self-knowledge, but talk of 'self knowledge' is ambiguous. On the one hand, as
we find in CT, it can refer to an intellectual achievement: of
coming to know such and such facts about our own psychological functioning.
Such knowledge can with luck and courage be put to instrumental use: we learn
to identify our cognitive pitfalls and - guided by enhanced skills in rational
self-challenge, self-care and self-management - reform the unruly mind and free
ourselves from psychological ruts. On the other hand, as found in
psychoanalytic psychotherapy, it can mean what we could call an ethical achievement:
of taking responsibility, mustering authority, cultivating character, and
acknowledging - in a moral rather than factual sense - one’s thoughts and
feelings. Coming to know one’s own mind, in this sense, is not of instrumental
but rather of intrinsic value: it already means not a cognizance, but the
relinquishing, of one’s defences, and coming to live as an increasingly
self-possessed, integrated subject. The intended result is not that one comes
to speak more knowledgeably about one’s thoughts and feelings, nor
better control the recalcitrant denizens of the mind, but rather that one can
now identify with and speak directly from them; not
correctness but truthfulness is its goal.
CT understands itself as a technology to, in the
words of a popular self-help CT book, help you ‘manage your mind’ (Butler &
Hope, 2007). As a development from a variant of psychoanalysis which offered
merely intellectual insight it is surely to be commended. As he describes it,
Beck moved away from offering hypotheses about his patient’s unconscious which,
whether they were right or wrong, were no match for his taking a real interest
in her experience of being herself. Furthermore, intellectual self-knowledge,
and the self-management it instrumentally makes for, do have an obvious if
limited value, as too do the warmth and honesty of a good therapist (Ablon
& Jones 1999), and the behavioural and experiential elements that provide
the 'B' in contemporary CBT (Jacobson et al 1996). The question I have
raised in this paper is whether psychotherapy can’t be very much more than
this, and whether CT’s dismissal of the psychopathogenic and psychotherapeutic
significance of unconscious motivation and of working in the transference might
jeopardize its ability to provide that something more. In particular, without
the patient coming to recognise, own and relinquish her attempts to defensively
avoid her feelings in the relationship with the psychotherapist, the clear risk
is of supplementing an already exhausting project of defensively managing
intolerable feelings with the further project of cognitively managing their
symptomatic products. The result may be temporary symptomatic improvement but the
cost is longer-term characterological and developmental stasis.
References
Ablon JS. Jones EE. (1999).
Psychotherapy process in the National Institute of Mental Health Treatment of
Depression Collaborative Research Program. Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology, 67:64–75.
Andersen, S.M., and Chen, S. (2002). The relational
self: An interpersonal social-cognitive theory. Psychological Review,
109, 619-645.
Barrett, Deirdre & Loeffler, M. (1992)
Comparison of dream content of depressed vs non-depressed dreamers. Psychological
Reports, 70, pp. 403-406.
Beck, A & Beck, J. (2012). Aaron T. Beck,
M.D. Interviewed by Judith S. Beck, Ph.D., Beck Institute for Cognitive
Behavior Therapy. Available: https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=7BZp7ZiAE3c
[accessed 23.8.2015]
Beck, A. T., & Hurvich, M. S. (1959). Psychological
correlates of depression: 1. Frequency of “masochistic” dream content in a
private practice sample. Psychosomatic Medicine, 21, 50-55.
Beck, A. T. (1967) Depression:
Clinical, Experimental, and Theoretical Aspects. Philadelphila: University of Pennsylvania Press.
Beck, A. T., & Ward, C. H.
(1961). Dreams of depressed patients: Characteristic themes in manifest
content. Archives of General Psychiatry, 5, 462-467.
Beck, Judith S. (2010) Cognitive
Behavior Therapy: Myths and Realities. Huffington Post. Posted
7.11.2010. Retrieved
11.9.2015. http://www.huffingtonpost.com/judith-s-beck-phd/cognitive-behavior-therap_b_638396.html
Fredric
N. Busch. 2009. Anger and depression, Advances in Psychiatric Treatment,
15 (4) 271-278.
Butler, Gillian and Hope, Tony.
(2007). Manage your mind. 2nd edition. Oxford: Oxford University
Press.
Clark, D. A., Beck, A. T. & Alford, B. A. (1999). Scientific
foundations of cognitive theory and therapy of depression. New York, NY:
John Wiley & Sons.
Albert Ellis (1962). Reason and
emotion in psychotherapy. New York: Lyle Stuart.
Jon Frederickson, 1999. Psychodynamic
Psychotherapy. Philadelphia: Brunner/Mazel.
Jon
Frederickson. 2013. Co-Creating
Change: Effective Dynamic Therapy Techniques. Kansas City: Seven Leaves
Press.
Sigmund Freud 1913. The
Interpretation of Dreams. New York: Macmillan.
Sigmund Freud 1917 Mourning and Melancholia. SE 14,
239-258.
Fried D; Crits-Christoph P; Luborsky L. (1992). The
first empirical demonstration of transference in psychotherapy. The
Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 180 (5), pp. 326-31
Jacobson, N. S., K. S. Dobson, P. A. Truax et al.
(1996). A component analysis of cognitive behavioural treatment for
depression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64, 2,
295-304.
Jonathan Lear 2014. Integrating the non-rational
soul. Proceedings of the Aristotelian Society, 114, 1, 75-101.
Trevor Lubbe. 2011. Object Relations in
Depression: A Return to Theory. Hove: Routledge.
Shedler, Jonathan. 2015. The therapy relationship
in psychodynamic therapy versus CBT. Psychologically Minded. Posted
18.3.2015. Retrieved
11.9.2015. https://www.psychologytoday.com/blog/psychologically-minded/201503/the-therapy-relationship-in-psychodynamic-therapy-versus-cbt
Comments
Post a Comment
Comment here!